Hello there, future psych whiz! It's time to delve into the wonderful, albeit sometimes puzzling, world of psychology research. But worry not, I am here to be your friendly guide as we tackle the concepts of credibility, generalizability, and validity in experiments.
First, let's make sure we understand two big words – 'credibility' and 'generalizability'. These terms are like quality control inspectors of research studies. They ensure that a study is not just an academic exercise, but has real value and applicability. Think of them like a teacher's red pen, making sure your homework is top notch.
Now, when it comes to experiments, we usually talk about three types of validity - construct, internal, and external.
Construct Validity: This is all about how well your experiment translates a theoretical concept (a construct) into something you can actually observe and measure (operationalization).
Let's think of an example. Say we want to measure the level of 'anxiety'. Anxiety itself is a theoretical concept that we can't directly see or touch. So, we need to find a way to operationalize it, to make it measurable. Some researchers tried to measure anxiety by using a 'fidgetometer' (yeah, you heard it right!), a chair that measures how much a person fidgets. The idea was simple: the more anxious you are, the more you fidget. But does this really cover the whole idea of 'anxiety'? Well, that's a big question for construct validity.
Internal Validity: This is about how confident we are that the changes in our independent variable (IV) really caused changes in the dependent variable (DV), and it's not just some other random factor playing tricks on us.
Suppose in our 'fidgetometer' experiment, we find that people fidget more after drinking coffee. Can we confidently say that it was the coffee (our IV) causing the increase in fidgeting (our DV)? Or was it perhaps the cold temperature in the room that made them shiver and move around more? If we can rule out these extra variables, our experiment has high internal validity.
External Validity: This relates to how well the findings from our experiment can be applied to the real world. There are two subtypes here - population validity and ecological validity.
Population validity is about whether we can apply our findings to a larger group of people (population) beyond our sample. For instance, if our 'fidgetometer' study was only conducted with teenagers, can we confidently say that the findings apply to adults too? If the answer is yes, we have high population validity.
Ecological validity, on the other hand, is about whether our experiment's findings can be applied to real-life settings. Say our 'fidgetometer' study was conducted in a very sterile, silent room. Would the same results hold if we conducted the study in a noisy café or a crowded park? If yes, we've scored well on ecological validity.
Dive deeper and gain exclusive access to premium files of Psychology SL. Subscribe now and get closer to that 45 🌟
Hello there, future psych whiz! It's time to delve into the wonderful, albeit sometimes puzzling, world of psychology research. But worry not, I am here to be your friendly guide as we tackle the concepts of credibility, generalizability, and validity in experiments.
First, let's make sure we understand two big words – 'credibility' and 'generalizability'. These terms are like quality control inspectors of research studies. They ensure that a study is not just an academic exercise, but has real value and applicability. Think of them like a teacher's red pen, making sure your homework is top notch.
Now, when it comes to experiments, we usually talk about three types of validity - construct, internal, and external.
Construct Validity: This is all about how well your experiment translates a theoretical concept (a construct) into something you can actually observe and measure (operationalization).
Let's think of an example. Say we want to measure the level of 'anxiety'. Anxiety itself is a theoretical concept that we can't directly see or touch. So, we need to find a way to operationalize it, to make it measurable. Some researchers tried to measure anxiety by using a 'fidgetometer' (yeah, you heard it right!), a chair that measures how much a person fidgets. The idea was simple: the more anxious you are, the more you fidget. But does this really cover the whole idea of 'anxiety'? Well, that's a big question for construct validity.
Internal Validity: This is about how confident we are that the changes in our independent variable (IV) really caused changes in the dependent variable (DV), and it's not just some other random factor playing tricks on us.
Suppose in our 'fidgetometer' experiment, we find that people fidget more after drinking coffee. Can we confidently say that it was the coffee (our IV) causing the increase in fidgeting (our DV)? Or was it perhaps the cold temperature in the room that made them shiver and move around more? If we can rule out these extra variables, our experiment has high internal validity.
External Validity: This relates to how well the findings from our experiment can be applied to the real world. There are two subtypes here - population validity and ecological validity.
Population validity is about whether we can apply our findings to a larger group of people (population) beyond our sample. For instance, if our 'fidgetometer' study was only conducted with teenagers, can we confidently say that the findings apply to adults too? If the answer is yes, we have high population validity.
Ecological validity, on the other hand, is about whether our experiment's findings can be applied to real-life settings. Say our 'fidgetometer' study was conducted in a very sterile, silent room. Would the same results hold if we conducted the study in a noisy café or a crowded park? If yes, we've scored well on ecological validity.
Dive deeper and gain exclusive access to premium files of Psychology SL. Subscribe now and get closer to that 45 🌟